The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality

Interoception is defined as an afferent information that can be sensed anywhere from within the body. Interoceptive accuracy is an objective measure of detection of interoceptive signals. According to previous studies the differences between humans in experiencing intence emotions lies in thei...

Täydet tiedot

Bibliografiset tiedot
Päätekijät: Kouvo, Noora, Puustinen, Jenni
Muut tekijät: Yhteiskuntatieteellinen tiedekunta, Faculty of Social Sciences, Psykologian laitos, Department of Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylän yliopisto
Aineistotyyppi: Pro gradu
Kieli:eng
Julkaistu: 2016
Aiheet:
Linkit: https://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/52588
_version_ 1828193109724364800
author Kouvo, Noora Puustinen, Jenni
author2 Yhteiskuntatieteellinen tiedekunta Faculty of Social Sciences Psykologian laitos Department of Psychology University of Jyväskylä Jyväskylän yliopisto
author_facet Kouvo, Noora Puustinen, Jenni Yhteiskuntatieteellinen tiedekunta Faculty of Social Sciences Psykologian laitos Department of Psychology University of Jyväskylä Jyväskylän yliopisto Kouvo, Noora Puustinen, Jenni Yhteiskuntatieteellinen tiedekunta Faculty of Social Sciences Psykologian laitos Department of Psychology University of Jyväskylä Jyväskylän yliopisto
author_sort Kouvo, Noora
datasource_str_mv jyx
description Interoception is defined as an afferent information that can be sensed anywhere from within the body. Interoceptive accuracy is an objective measure of detection of interoceptive signals. According to previous studies the differences between humans in experiencing intence emotions lies in their ability to accurately detect interoceptive signals. It seems that temperament and personality could be behind the differences as there is evidence that especially temperament has connection to emotionality and can be connected to neural systems that are also linked to interoception. We hypothesized that temperament is more likely to be related to interoception than personality and that temperament traits related to negative affectivity and anxiety are related to interoceptive accuracy. This study was part of a project on interoception that began in University of Jyväskylä’s psychology faculty in the autumn of 2015. There were 37 participants aged between 18-35. The interoceptive accuracy was tested with experimental setting including a heartbeat detection task. Participants were asked to attend to their own heartbeat and answer if the delivered audio signal matched their real-time heartbeat or not. 16 series of auditory signals were delivered, each of them containing 10 sounds. Each of the sounds were either synchronized to participants’ heartbeat or were delivered with a 500-millisecond delay. Personality and temperament were measured with questionnaires. The data was analyzed statistically with independent sample t-test, Pearson correlation and regression analysis. As we expected, interoception and temperament were connected but interoception and personality were not. The strongest connection was between interoception and factor scale effortful control and especially its subscale attention control. The second important link appeared between interoception and orienting sensitivity and its subscale associative sensitivity. Sadness also appeared to have some kind of connection to interoception and it had an effect on the connection between interoception and the sensitivity traits. It remains unclear whether level of interoceptive accuracy causes certain temperament traits or the traits cause the accuracy.
first_indexed 2023-03-22T09:56:50Z
format Pro gradu
fullrecord [{"key": "dc.contributor.author", "value": "Kouvo, Noora", "language": null, "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "author", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.contributor.author", "value": "Puustinen, Jenni", "language": null, "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "author", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.date.accessioned", "value": "2017-01-03T07:32:19Z", "language": "", "element": "date", "qualifier": "accessioned", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.date.available", "value": "2017-01-03T07:32:19Z", "language": "", "element": "date", "qualifier": "available", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.date.issued", "value": "2016", "language": null, "element": "date", "qualifier": "issued", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.identifier.other", "value": "oai:jykdok.linneanet.fi:1645843", "language": null, "element": "identifier", "qualifier": "other", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.identifier.uri", "value": "https://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/52588", "language": "", "element": "identifier", "qualifier": "uri", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.description.abstract", "value": "Interoception is defined as an afferent information that can be sensed anywhere from within the \r\nbody. Interoceptive accuracy is an objective measure of detection of interoceptive \r\nsignals. According to previous studies the differences between humans in experiencing intence \r\nemotions lies in their ability to accurately detect interoceptive signals. It seems that temperament \r\nand personality could be behind the differences as there is evidence that especially temperament has \r\nconnection to emotionality and can be connected to neural systems that are also linked to \r\ninteroception. We hypothesized that temperament is more likely to be related to interoception than \r\npersonality and that temperament traits related to negative affectivity and anxiety are related to \r\ninteroceptive accuracy. \r\n\r\n \r\n\r\nThis study was part of a project on interoception that began in University of Jyv\u00e4skyl\u00e4\u2019s psychology \r\nfaculty in the autumn of 2015. There were 37 participants aged between 18-35. The interoceptive \r\naccuracy was tested with experimental setting including a heartbeat detection task. Participants \r\nwere asked to attend to their own heartbeat and answer if the delivered audio signal matched their \r\nreal-time heartbeat or not. 16 series of auditory signals were delivered, each of them containing 10 \r\nsounds. Each of the sounds were either synchronized to participants\u2019 heartbeat or were delivered \r\nwith a 500-millisecond delay. Personality and temperament were measured with questionnaires. \r\nThe data was analyzed statistically with independent sample t-test, Pearson correlation and \r\nregression analysis. \r\n\r\n \r\n\r\nAs we expected, interoception and temperament were connected but interoception and personality \r\nwere not. The strongest connection was between interoception and factor scale effortful control and \r\nespecially its subscale attention control. The second important link appeared between interoception \r\nand orienting sensitivity and its subscale associative sensitivity. Sadness also appeared to have \r\nsome kind of connection to interoception and it had an effect on the connection between \r\ninteroception and the sensitivity traits. It remains unclear whether level of interoceptive accuracy \r\ncauses certain temperament traits or the traits cause the accuracy.", "language": "en", "element": "description", "qualifier": "abstract", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.description.provenance", "value": "Submitted using Plone Publishing form by Miia Hakanen (mihakane) on 2017-01-03 07:32:18.870736. Form: Admin-lomake opinn\u00e4ytteiden julkaisuun (https://kirjasto.jyu.fi/julkaisut/julkaisulomakkeet/admin-lomake-opinnaytteet). JyX data: [jyx_publishing-allowed (fi) =False]", "language": "en", "element": "description", "qualifier": "provenance", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.description.provenance", "value": "Submitted by jyx lomake-julkaisija (jyx-julkaisija.group@korppi.jyu.fi) on 2017-01-03T07:32:19Z\r\nNo. of bitstreams: 2\r\nURN:NBN:fi:jyu-201701031037.pdf: 561388 bytes, checksum: d56b5ae9add3cd630a4c30bfe958c5e3 (MD5)\r\nlicense.html: 107 bytes, checksum: a7d86e598caa500b1b433bbb9dc8ef1c (MD5)", "language": "en", "element": "description", "qualifier": "provenance", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.description.provenance", "value": "Made available in DSpace on 2017-01-03T07:32:19Z (GMT). No. of bitstreams: 2\r\nURN:NBN:fi:jyu-201701031037.pdf: 561388 bytes, checksum: d56b5ae9add3cd630a4c30bfe958c5e3 (MD5)\r\nlicense.html: 107 bytes, checksum: a7d86e598caa500b1b433bbb9dc8ef1c (MD5)\r\n Previous issue date: 2016", "language": "en", "element": "description", "qualifier": "provenance", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.format.extent", "value": "1 verkkoaineisto (51 sivua)", "language": null, "element": "format", "qualifier": "extent", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.format.mimetype", "value": "application/pdf", "language": null, "element": "format", "qualifier": "mimetype", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.language.iso", "value": "eng", "language": null, "element": "language", "qualifier": "iso", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.rights", "value": "In Copyright", "language": "en", "element": "rights", "qualifier": null, "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.other", "value": "interoseptio", "language": null, "element": "subject", "qualifier": "other", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.other", "value": "kehoaisti", "language": null, "element": "subject", "qualifier": "other", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.title", "value": "The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality", "language": null, "element": "title", "qualifier": null, "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.type", "value": "master thesis", "language": null, "element": "type", "qualifier": null, "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.identifier.urn", "value": "URN:NBN:fi:jyu-201701031037", "language": null, "element": "identifier", "qualifier": "urn", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.type.ontasot", "value": "Pro gradu -tutkielma", "language": "fi", "element": "type", "qualifier": "ontasot", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.type.ontasot", "value": "Master\u2019s thesis", "language": "en", "element": "type", "qualifier": "ontasot", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.contributor.faculty", "value": "Yhteiskuntatieteellinen tiedekunta", "language": "fi", "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "faculty", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.contributor.faculty", "value": "Faculty of Social Sciences", "language": "en", "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "faculty", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.contributor.department", "value": "Psykologian laitos", "language": "fi", "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "department", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.contributor.department", "value": "Department of Psychology", "language": "en", "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "department", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.contributor.organization", "value": "University of Jyv\u00e4skyl\u00e4", "language": "en", "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "organization", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.contributor.organization", "value": "Jyv\u00e4skyl\u00e4n yliopisto", "language": "fi", "element": "contributor", "qualifier": "organization", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.discipline", "value": "Psykologia", "language": "fi", "element": "subject", "qualifier": "discipline", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.discipline", "value": "Psychology", "language": "en", "element": "subject", "qualifier": "discipline", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.date.updated", "value": "2017-01-03T07:32:19Z", "language": "", "element": "date", "qualifier": "updated", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "yvv.contractresearch.funding", "value": "0", "language": "", "element": "contractresearch", "qualifier": "funding", "schema": "yvv"}, {"key": "dc.type.coar", "value": "http://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_bdcc", "language": null, "element": "type", "qualifier": "coar", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.rights.accesslevel", "value": "restrictedAccess", "language": "fi", "element": "rights", "qualifier": "accesslevel", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.type.publication", "value": "masterThesis", "language": null, "element": "type", "qualifier": "publication", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.oppiainekoodi", "value": "202", "language": null, "element": "subject", "qualifier": "oppiainekoodi", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.yso", "value": "aistimukset", "language": null, "element": "subject", "qualifier": "yso", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.yso", "value": "temperamentti", "language": null, "element": "subject", "qualifier": "yso", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.yso", "value": "persoonallisuus", "language": null, "element": "subject", "qualifier": "yso", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.subject.yso", "value": "syke", "language": null, "element": "subject", "qualifier": "yso", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.format.content", "value": "fulltext", "language": null, "element": "format", "qualifier": "content", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.rights.url", "value": "https://rightsstatements.org/page/InC/1.0/", "language": null, "element": "rights", "qualifier": "url", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.rights.accessrights", "value": "Aineistoon p\u00e4\u00e4sy\u00e4 on rajoitettu tekij\u00e4noikeussyist\u00e4. Aineisto on luettavissa Jyv\u00e4skyl\u00e4n yliopiston kirjaston arkistoty\u00f6asemalta. Ks. https://kirjasto.jyu.fi/fi/tyoskentelytilat/laitteet-ja-tilat.", "language": "fi", "element": "rights", "qualifier": "accessrights", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.rights.accessrights", "value": "This material has a restricted access due to copyright reasons. It can be read at the workstation at Jyv\u00e4skyl\u00e4 University Library reserved for the use of archival materials: https://kirjasto.jyu.fi/en/workspaces/facilities.", "language": "en", "element": "rights", "qualifier": "accessrights", "schema": "dc"}, {"key": "dc.type.okm", "value": "G2", "language": null, "element": "type", "qualifier": "okm", "schema": "dc"}]
id jyx.123456789_52588
language eng
last_indexed 2025-03-31T20:00:52Z
main_date 2016-01-01T00:00:00Z
main_date_str 2016
publishDate 2016
record_format qdc
source_str_mv jyx
spellingShingle Kouvo, Noora Puustinen, Jenni The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality interoseptio kehoaisti Psykologia Psychology 202 aistimukset temperamentti persoonallisuus syke
title The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality
title_full The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality
title_fullStr The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality
title_full_unstemmed The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality
title_short The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality
title_sort relation between interoceptive accuracy temperament and personality
title_txtP The relation between interoceptive accuracy, temperament and personality
topic interoseptio kehoaisti Psykologia Psychology 202 aistimukset temperamentti persoonallisuus syke
topic_facet 202 Psychology Psykologia aistimukset interoseptio kehoaisti persoonallisuus syke temperamentti
url https://jyx.jyu.fi/handle/123456789/52588 http://www.urn.fi/URN:NBN:fi:jyu-201701031037
work_keys_str_mv AT kouvonoora relationbetweeninteroceptiveaccuracytemperamentandpersonality AT kouvonoora therelationbetweeninteroceptiveaccuracytemperamentandpersonality